David Brooks had an excellent column the other day on the "splendor of cities," how these massive agglomerations of people remain essential even in the age of Facebook, Facetime, Twitter and email. While many people predicted during the frenzy of the dot-com bubble that the rise of the internet would mean the "end of geography" - the zeroes and ones hurtling across the fiber optic cables should supply us with all the interactions we need - that hasn't happened. Instead, cities have become more important than ever. Here's Brooks:
Somewhere, Jane Jacobs is smiling. As I noted in a recent Times Magazine article, the physicists Geoffrey West and Luis Bettencourt have tried to quantify the productivity gains produced by cities. They began by data mining vast tracts of statistics released by various government agencies:
At this point, the science remains mostly statistical - I don't think anyone has a clear idea why density produces innovation, or why people who are physically close are so much more productive. As a result, we end up relying on vague words like "knowledge spillovers" or "knowledge trading" or "implicitly exchanged information." Nevertheless, the statistics are convincing. Consider a study published earlier this year by Isaac Kohane, a researcher at Harvard Medical School. Kohane’s question was simple: How does physical proximity affect the quality of scientific research? To answer this question, he analyzed more than 35,000 different peer-reviewed papers, mapping the precise location of every single co-author. Once the data was amassed, the correlation became clear: When co-authors were located closer together, their papers tended to be of significantly higher quality, at least as measured by the number of subsequent citations. The best research was consistently produced when scientists were working within ten meters of each other, while the worst papers tended to emerge from collaborators located a kilometer or more apart. (On average, a paper with four or fewer authors located in the same building was cited 45 percent more than if the authors were in different buildings.) For whatever reason, electronic interactions are not (at least not yet) a substitute for the real world; Facetime can't compete with a real face. As the work of Glaeser and Kohane demonstrates, our most important new ideas typically don’t arrive on a screen. Rather, they emerge from idle conversation, from too many people sharing the same space.